首页
外语
计算机
考研
公务员
职业资格
财经
工程
司法
医学
专升本
自考
实用职业技能
登录
外语
Should A Kid Be Guided to Tell a White Lie? It’s my family’s tradition to exchange girls on Christmas Eve. Before we did so,
Should A Kid Be Guided to Tell a White Lie? It’s my family’s tradition to exchange girls on Christmas Eve. Before we did so,
admin
2010-07-24
60
问题
Should A Kid Be Guided to Tell a White Lie?
It’s my family’s tradition to exchange girls on Christmas Eve. Before we did so, I whispered to my uncle and his wife, "Just want you to know: I think what I got you is really cool, so just tell me you like it, no matter what, okay?"
I know that sounds rude, but there’s another Christmas custom in my family: we give each other weird gifts.
There is a sweet reason for this. My grandparents grew up during the Depression, and there were years when they had no gifts at all. So my grandmother and her siblings(兄弟姐妹) would gift-wrap their old socks and clothes, just so they had something to open on Christmas. Pretend presents were better than none at all.
My grandmother never really got over those early years, so, for the rest of her life, she went a little crazy at the holidays. She’d start buying gifts in October. It didn’t matter what it was. Socks, toothbrushes, used paperbacks she’d read but didn’t like, all went under the tree. Contents of catalog "mystery boxes" meant we spent another hour unwrapping presents. One of my more memorable gifts: a single piece of clear plastic labeled "face shield." I was apparently to hold it in front of my eyes when I used hair spray.
We all thanked Grandma greatly no matter what we got. As a little kid, this pattern of gratitude for the terrible presents puzzled me; it took a long time for me to understand it was all right to laugh at some of her gifts. Now I don’t really know if my aunt and uncle actually liked the gift I gave them. They said they did, but since I coached them to tell me they love it, I’ll never really know if that was the truth.
All of these make me think of the work of McGill professor Victoria Talwar. As an expert in children’s lying behavior, Talwar has been studying how kids respond to unwanted gifts. When they get a gift they hate, can they still thank someone and pretend to love it?
Talwar tests kids’ ability to do this, by asking kids to pick a toy they want; if they win a game, they get the chosen toy. There are plastic horses, a small car, a few other items, including an unwrapped, dirty, worn, used bar of soap. At some point in the game, there’s a switch in the adults who play with the kids. So, instead of giving the child her chosen toy, the late-arriving adult gives the child the soap.
Then, the researchers watch what happens. 68% of kids, aged 3 to 11, will spontaneously say they love the gift of old ugly soap. The older they are, the more likely they are to say a white lie about the gift. And if parents encourage the children to say how much they like the present, the percentage of kids lying about the gift increases to 87%.
At this point, some may be saying that a white lie isn’t a lie. That’s because you are looking at lying from the adult perspective--that lies are acceptable, when told with the intent of helping someone, or protecting another’s feelings. But kids don’t think of lying in the same way. For them, the intent behind a lie--for good or for ill--is irrelevant. It is so irrelevant that, for very young kids, you can’t even lie by accident. Someone who gives out wrong information, but believed it to be true, is still a liar in these kids’ book.
Kids just don’t believe that lying comes in shades of white or gray. Lying is much simpler than that: lying is telling somebody something that isn’t so; lying is really bad; and lying gets you punished. And if it gets you punished, you shouldn’t do it. In Talwar’s lab, parents have literally cheered to hear their kids lie about how great it is to have received the old soap. The parents have pride over their children’s knowing the socially appropriate response.
Talwar’s regularly amazed by this. The parents never even seem to realize that the child told a lie. They never want to scold the child afterwards, or talk about the kid’s behavior.
Regardless of the parents’ pride, the kids aren’t happy about their successfully lying. Instead, it can be torture for them.
I was at Talwar’s lab when she was doing a version of the unwanted gift experiment with kids in the first and second grades. Watching kid after kid react to that gross bar of soap, I could really see how emotionally difficult it is for kids to tell a white lie. The kids were disappointed when they were handed the soap, but that was nothing compared to the discomfort they showed while having to lie about liking it. They are uneasy. Some looked like they were going to cry. It was simply painful to watch. Indeed, Talwar has found that some kids just can’t even bring themselves to say something nice about the present. About 20% of 11-year-olds just refuse to tell a white lie about that unwanted gift--even after their parents encouraged them to do so. And about 14% of kids still won’t tell a white lie, even after their parents specifically explained the prosocial (亲社会的) reasons to tell the lie. These kids just can’t reconcile the disconnect between knowing how bad lying is, and being told they should now lie. Talwar cautions that we need to recognize that, at least from the kids’ point of view, white lies really are still lies. We should take care to explain the motivation behind the untruth--that we want to protect the other person’s feelings. Kids may still fail to completely understand the distinction, but at least it will encourage them to think about others’ feelings when they act. And we need to reassure children that they won’t be punished for a specific white lie--because they did something nice for someone else.
Talwar also warns that we adults should pay attention to our own use of white lies. Kids notice these untruths-and that we rarely get punished for them. If kids believe that we regularly lie to get out of uncomfortable social situations, they are more likely to adopt a similar strategy of lying. If we don’t watch it, we could unintentionally be giving kids yet another present: a license to lie.
What’s the purpose of Talwar’s study?
选项
A、To see how kids respond to unwanted gifts.
B、To test kids’ ability to refuse their dislikes.
C、To study how parents react to kids’ lies.
D、To check how polite the kids are.
答案
A
解析
该句how 从句是study(研究)的内容,纵观全文可知也是研究的目的,A与此相符,故选A。
转载请注明原文地址:https://www.kaotiyun.com/show/o0N7777K
0
大学英语四级
相关试题推荐
Forthispart,youareallowed30minutestowriteacompositionabouttherelationshipbetweenwealthandhappiness.Youshould
Itisdifficulttoimaginewhatlifewouldbelikewithoutmemory.Themeaningsofthousandsofeveryday【S1】______,thebasesfo
Althoughthepossibilityoflivingalongandhappylifenowadaysisgreaterthaneverbefore,everydaywewitnesstheincredib
PreparingforTestsIdeallyitshouldbeloveoflearning,achievement,andself-improvementthatpromptsalllearning.But
AmericanEnglishisanothervarietyoftheEnglishlanguagespokenintheUnitedStates.AlthoughallAmericansdonotspeakthe
A、Stayathome.B、Gototravelagenciesformoreinformation.C、GotoHawaiiforatravel.D、Turntootherfriendsforabetter
Internetvotinghappensallthetime,butusuallyit’sconfinedtotopicssuchas"WhoisthecutestcastmemberofPartyofFiv
A、Heknowssomeonewhocangetthewomanajob.B、Gregandhisuncledon’tliketheirjobs.C、Gregonlygotthejobbecauseofh
TheUnitedStatesisacountrymadeupofmanydifferentraces.Usuallytheyaremixedtogetherandcan’tbetoldonefromanoth
A、Heworksinarepairshop.B、Heisabouttoretire.C、Heisgoingtoopenastore.D、Heloveshispresentjob.B本题为推理题。男士回答:“Y
随机试题
电路元件中主要有电阻元件、电感元件、电容元件和电源元件等。()
采用“一人多事”和“冰糖葫芦”式结构的《陈毅市长》是【】
中枢兴奋药的特征是:
患者,女,45岁。蛋白尿3年,尿少3天入院,全身凹陷性水肿,血压正常,血白蛋白20g/L,总胆固醇6.35mmol/L,肾功能正常,尿蛋白6g/24h,患者最可能的诊断是()
重度支气管哮喘最主要的治疗药物是
在组织流水施工时,异节奏流水施工的基本特点包括()。
将可转换债券视为一般的债券所具有价值称为()
设计:修建:高楼
根据下列材料回答问题。2008年1至3月份,福建沿海地区与台湾地区海上客运直航船舶的公司共运营2425航次,共运送旅客188648人次,分别为2007年同期的108%、97%;其中,大陆5家船舶公司经营的7艘客船共运营1190航次,运送旅客960
如果二氧化碳气体超量产生,就会在大气层中聚集,使全球气候出现令人讨厌的温室效应。在绿色植被覆盖的地方,特别是在森林中,通过光合作用,绿色植物吸收空气中的二氧化碳,放出氧气。因此,从这个意义上,绿色植被特别是森林的破坏,就意味着在“生产”二氧化碳。工厂中对由
最新回复
(
0
)