首页
外语
计算机
考研
公务员
职业资格
财经
工程
司法
医学
专升本
自考
实用职业技能
登录
外语
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilization thinkers have usually answered either that it come
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilization thinkers have usually answered either that it come
admin
2015-01-09
57
问题
Where does morality come from? Throughout the history of Western civilization thinkers have usually answered either that it comes from God, or else through the application of reason.
But in The Bonobo and the Atheist, primatologist Frans de Waal argues that there’s another answer that fits the data better: morality comes from our evolutionary past as a social primate. Like our closest relatives, the apes, humans evolved in small, tightly knit, cooperative groups. As a result, again like the apes, we are exquisitely sensitive to one another’s moods, needs and intentions.
This well-developed empathy provided the trellis on which morality later flowered. De Waal, who is based at Emory University in Atlanta, Georgia, has been making this case eloquently for many years and over several books, notably in Good Natured back in 1997, and in Primates and Philosophers, 12 years later.
In his new work, he bolsters the argument by drawing on a lot of new research, carefully footnoted for those who want to dig deeper. De Waal distinguishes two degrees of morality. The first he calls "one-on-one morality", which governs how an individual can expect to be treated, and the second "community concern", a larger, more abstract concept that extends to the harmony of the group as a whole.
Chimps and bonobos certainly have the former—they respect ownership, for example, and expect to be treated according to their place in the hierarchy. But de Waal presents several examples—such as a chimp stepping in to stop a fight between two others—that suggest that they also have a rudimentary form of the latter.
The book’s title, incidentally, draws on bonobos because they are more likely than chimps to behave morally, to have concern for each other, to value harmony and so on. This, imagines, de Waal, is something morally inclined atheists would want to emulate. If humans inherited morality from our ancestors, though, what are we to make of religion? Here de Waal moves into the territory he has not explored before. Clearly, religion must do something important, since every human culture has it. But instead of religion giving us morality, de Waal turns the tables. Morality, he argues, probably gave us religion as a way of reinforcing the pre-existing community concern.
If he’s right, then there may be no absolute code of right and wrong out there to be discovered. Instead, each individual’s evolved sense of empathy and concern for the group may help shape the group’s consensus on what kind of behaviour is appropriate. In short, says de Waal, morality may be something we all have to work out together. It’s a persuasive argument, and de Waal’s cautious and evidence-based approach is one that many New Scientist readers are sure to find congenial.
That careful approach is less evident in another book covering some of the same ground. In How Animals Grieve, anthropologist Barbara King sets out to explore the question of whether non-human animals grieve for their dead. It’s an intriguing question, but unfortunately King’s book is largely a succession of anecdotes: the cat who roams the house, crying, in search of its dead litter mate; the dog who waits daily at the train station for its dead master; a dolphin trying to keep her dead calf afloat for days.
Some of these stories make a persuasive case for some animals—especially apes, elephants and cetaceans— sometimes grieving. No surprises there: I suspect most readers would have conceded that ground right from the start.
But King makes little effort to dig any deeper by exploring, for example, the neural machinery and cognitive skills an animal needs in order to be capable of grief. After all, solitary species such as cats have less need for empathy- and its corollary, grief—than social animals, and small-brained creatures such as turtles may simply lack the brainpower or not form lasting pair bonds.
To his credit, de Waal takes full note of such distinctions; King, not so much.
Which statement would Frans de Waal most probably agree to?
选项
A、Bonobos are not as concerned about each other as chimps.
B、Atheists would like to emulate the harmony among Bonobos.
C、Bonobos are a good example to show where morality comes from.
D、Atheists are a group of people who are lack of moral standards.
答案
C
解析
细节题。作者在第六段第一句提到Frans de Waal的书时指出,这本书的名字用倭黑猩猩(bonobos)是因为它们比黑猩猩(chimps)在行为上表现得更道德,故可知,Frans de Waal认为倭黑猩猩是说明道德来源的好例子,故[C]为答案。
转载请注明原文地址:https://www.kaotiyun.com/show/h5dO777K
0
专业英语八级
相关试题推荐
WhichofthefollowingstatementsisINCORRECTaboutMissChan?
Accordingtothenewsitem,"sleepboxes"aredesignedtosolvetheproblemsof
Decadesago,wemadepenpalsbywritingletterstoeachother.Today,peoplearemorelikelytomakefriendsviatheinstantme
LondonissteepedinDickensianhistory.Everyplacehevisited,everypersonhemet,wouldbedrawnintohisimaginationandre
Changesinthewaypeoplelivebringaboutchangesinthejobsthattheydo.Moroandmorepeopleliveintownsandcitiesinst
ModernExaminationsIntheschoolsofancienttimes,themostimportantexaminationswerespoken.Usuallythestudentsweres
Engaginginahobbylikereadingabook,makingapatchworkquiltorevenplayingcomputergamescandelaytheonsetofdementi
ChinaisconsideringmakingitalegaldutyforChinesecitizenstovisittheiragedparentsasthecountrystruggleswiththec
对生命没有寄托的人,青年时代和“儿时”对他格外宝贵。这种罗曼蒂克的回忆其实并不是发现了“儿时”的真正了不得,而是感觉到“中年”以后的衰退。本来,生命只有一次,对于谁都是宝贵的。但是,假使他的生命溶化在大众里面,假使他天天在为这世界干些什么,那么,他总在生长
BesidesFrenchwords,Englishalsoabsorbedasmanyas2,500wordsof_____intheMiddleEnglishperiod.
随机试题
A.体液B.细胞内液C.细胞外液D.血浆机体的内环境是指
连续分齿断续展成飞刀铣削蜗轮时,若蜗轮齿数z2能被蜗杆头数z,除尽,那么一次能()。
对涂敷完成的3PE防腐管进行检测,以下不属于在线检测的项目是()。
金河秋半虏弦开,云外惊飞四散哀。仙掌月明孤影过,长门灯暗数声来。须知胡骑纷纷在,岂逐春风一一回。莫厌潇湘少人处,水多菰米岸莓苔。这首诗主要表达了诗人怎样的感情?
下列选项中,不是民法渊源的是()
某法院在判决书中以盗窃罪判处甲有期徒刑8年,以强奸罪判处甲有期徒刑10年,决定执行有期徒刑15年。这15年属于:()
费用控制的基础是()。
采用列表比较法时,发现工作现有总时差小于原有总时差,且仍有非负值,则说明()。
阅读材料,回答问题。材料一成书于两汉时期的数学著作《九章算术》,所依据的底稿是“秦汉之计籍”。它所涉及的方田、粟米、衰分、少广、商功、均输、盈不足、方程、勾股等范畴,都与当时生产生活实际直接相关,其运算技巧虽然高超,但全然无理论色彩可言。魏晋南北
陈述性知识学习的一般过程包括()
最新回复
(
0
)