首页
外语
计算机
考研
公务员
职业资格
财经
工程
司法
医学
专升本
自考
实用职业技能
登录
外语
Preschoolers’ Innate Knowledge Means They Can Probably Do Algebra Child development specialists are uncovering evidence that
Preschoolers’ Innate Knowledge Means They Can Probably Do Algebra Child development specialists are uncovering evidence that
admin
2015-03-28
55
问题
Preschoolers’ Innate Knowledge Means They Can Probably Do Algebra
Child development specialists are uncovering evidence that toddlers may understand much more than we think.
[A] Give a three-year old a smart phone and she’ll likely figure out how to turn it on and operate a few simple functions. But confront her with an algebra problem and ask her to solve for x? Not likely.
[B] For decades, child developmental psychologist Jean Piaget convinced us that young, undeveloped minds couldn’t handle complex concepts because they simply weren’t experienced or mature enough yet. Piaget, in fact, believed that toddlers could not understand cause and effect, that they couldn’t think logically, and that they also couldn’t handle abstract ideas.
[C] That’s because, he argued, children learn to develop these higher skills through trial and error. But child development specialists are finding out that preschoolers without any formal education may have the capacity to understand more complex concepts than we give them credit for, such as complicated rules for operating a toy or even solving for an unknown in algebra. Some of this is due to their ability to be more open and flexible about their world than adults. But beyond that, toddlers may have the innate ability to understand abstract concepts like quantities and causality, and that’s fueling an exciting stream of experiments that reveal just how sophisticated preschoolers’ brains might be.
[D] Alison Gopnik, professor of psychology at University of California Berkeley and her team devised a way to test how well young kids understand the abstract concept of multiple causality — the idea that there may be more than one cause for a single effect. They pitted 32 preschoolers around 4 years old against 143 un-dergrads. The study centered around a toy that could be turned on by placing a single blue-colored block on the toy’s tray, but could also be activated if two blocks of different colors — orange and purple — were placed on the tray. Both the kids and the undergraduates were shown how the toy worked and then asked which blocks activated the toy.
[E] The preschoolers were adept at figuring out that the blue blocks turned on the toy, as did the purple and orange ones. The Berkeley undergraduates, however, had a harder time accepting the scenario.Their previous experience in the world, which tends to work in a single-cause-equals-single-effect way, hampered their ability to accept the unusual rules that activated they toy; they wanted to believe that it was activated either by a single color or by a combination of colors, but not both.
[F] The preschoolers’ lack of bias about causality likely contributed to their ability to learn the multiple ways to activate the toy, but the results also suggest that preschoolers really can think logically and in more complicated ways. Just because they can’t express themselves or aren’t as adept at demonstrating such knowledge, doesn’t mean they don’t have it.
[G] Researchers from Johns Hopkins University, for example, found a similar effect among preschoolers when it came to math. Previous studies showed that if you present infants with eight objects over and over until they got bored, and then showed them 16, they suddenly regained interest and sensed that things changed. "All the evidence so far leads us to believe that this is something that babies come into the world with," says Melissa Kibbe, co-author of that study.
[H] She and her colleague Lisa Feigenson wondered if that innate sense of quantity might translate into an understanding of numbers and higher math functions, including solving for unknowns — one of the foundations of algebra — which often isn’t taught until seventh or eighth grades. So they conducted a series of experiments using a cup with a fixed amount of objects that substituted for x in the equation 5 + x=17.
[I] To divert the four- and six-year olds’ attention away from Arabic numerals to quantities instead, the researchers used a puppet and a "magic" cup that contained 12 buttons. In one of the experiments, the children saw five buttons on the table. After watching the researchers add the 12 buttons from the cup, they were told there were 17 buttons on the table. In another test, the youngsters saw three piles of objects — buttons, coins or small toys — in varying amounts, and observed the researchers adding the fixed number of contents of the puppet’s cup to each.
[J] After training the kids on how the cup worked, the researchers tried to confuse them with another cup containing fewer (such as four) or more (such as 24) objects. However, the kids understood intuitively that the decoy cup contained the wrong amount of items and that a specific amount — x, the "magic" cup amount — had to be added to reach the sum. That suggested that the preschoolers had some concept of quantity. What surprised Kibbe was not just that preschoolers understood the concept of adding "more," but that they could also calibrate how much more was needed to fill in the unknown quantity.
[K] "These kids had very little formal schooling so far, but what we are finding is that when we tap into their gut sense, something we call the Approximate Number Sense (ANS), kids are able to do much more complex calculations than if we gave them numbers and letters," says Kibbe of her results. And there doesn’t seem to be any gender differences in this innate ability, at least not among the girls and boys Kibbe studied.
[L] There’s also precedent for such innate pre-learning in reading, says Jon Star, at the Harvard University Graduate School of Education. To improve reading skills, some teachers have tapped into children’s memorization skills to make the connection between words and meaning more efficient.
[M] Kibbe’s and Gopnik’s recent work may have broader implications for education, since current math curricula in schools, which focuses on teaching Arabic numerals and on solving equations, may not be ideal for nurturing the number sense that kids are born with. "There’s an exciting movement in psychology over the past decade, as we learn that students bring certain capabilities, or innate knowledge that we hadn’t thought they had before," says Star.
[N] Though it may be too early to translate such findings to the classroom, the results lay the groundwork for studying similar innate skills and how they might be better understood. ANS, for example, is one of many constructs that young children may have that could enhance their learning but that current curricula aren’t exploiting. Developmental experts are still trying to figure out how malleable these constructs are, and how much of an impact they can have on future learning. For instance, do kids who hone their ANS skills become better at algebra and calculus in high school? "We still need to figure out which constructs matter most, and which are most amenable to interventions to help children improve their learning," says Star.
[O] "The hard part is, educationally, how do you build up and upon this intuitive knowledge in a way that allows a child to capture the complexity but not hold them back," says Tina Grotzer, associate professor of education at Harvard. Tapping into a child’s still developing sense of numbers and quantities is one thing, but overloading it with too many new constructs about algebra, unknowns, and problem solving may just gum up the working memory and end up adversely affecting his learning and academic performance.
[P] Still, that doesn’t mean that these innate skills shouldn’t be explored and possibly exploited in the class-room. Preschoolers may be smarter than we think, but we still have to figure out how to give them the right opportunities in the classroom so they know what to do with that knowledge.
Too many new constructs about algebra, unknowns, and problem solving may have some negative effects on a child’s learning.
选项
答案
O
解析
段末句提到,如果过多地充斥代数、未知数和问题解决方面的新构念,人的记忆功能可能就会变得混乱,并最终对孩子的学习和学业成绩产生不利影响。由此可见,过多地充斥代数、未知数和问题解决方面的新构念可能会对孩子的学业产生消极影响。本题是对该信息的转述。题中的have some negative effects对应文中的adversely affcting。
转载请注明原文地址:https://www.kaotiyun.com/show/Jeh7777K
0
大学英语六级
相关试题推荐
HowExerciseCouldLeadtoaBetterBrainA)Thevalueofmental-traininggamesmaybespeculative,asDanHurleywritesinhisa
HowExerciseCouldLeadtoaBetterBrainA)Thevalueofmental-traininggamesmaybespeculative,asDanHurleywritesinhisa
Forthispart,youareallowed30minutestowriteashortessayentitledOnDialectalTVPrograms.Youshouldwriteatleast15
Theymayappeartobemarvelsofmodernscience.Buttheideasthatledtothewondersofsatellite【B1】______,organtransplant
Amongthegovernment’smostinterestingreportsisonethatestimateswhatparentsspendontheirchildren.Notsurprisingly,th
Justoveradecadeintothe21stcentury,women’sprogresscanbecelebratedacrossarangeoffields.Theyholdthehighestpol
A、Ithasadensepopulation.B、Ithasmanytoweringbuildings.C、Thereare200vehiclesforeverykilometerofroadway.D、There
A、ThedoctorcanseethemanonThursday.B、AppointmentsmustbemadeafterThursday.C、Themanmayrescheduleafterthedoctor
AttendParents’MeetingwithaGoodImageDirections:Forthispart,youareallowed30minutestowriteashortessayentitled
Mytopicishandedness—whetherindifferentsportsitisbettertobeleftorright-sidedorwhetheramorebalancedapproachis
随机试题
A.Hegar征B.MOntgOmery结节C.BraxtOnHicks收缩D.Meigs征E.仰卧位低血压综合征
需要进行消防设计的建筑工程,建设单位应当将建筑工程的消防设计图纸及有关资料报送( )审核。
质量手册包括工程产品质量形成全过程应控制的所有质量职能活动的内容,这体现了质量手册的( )。
消防水泵接合器的检查方法和技术要求包括()。
下列公司拟申请在全国股转系统挂牌,不符合挂牌条件的有()。[2017年6月真题]Ⅰ.甲公司实际控制人为张某、李某、王某三人,张某任公司董事长、李某任公司总经理、王某任财务负责人,三人在最近12个月内受到刑事处罚Ⅱ.乙公司为节约管理费
国务院子2000年公布并于2001年1月1日施行的《企业财务会计报告条例》,要求国有企业、国有控股的或占主导地位的企业应当在至少每年一次的职工代表大会上公布财务会计报告,并重点说明注册会计师的情况。( )
《物权法》规定,()是本建筑物或者建筑区划内所有建筑物的业主大会的执行机构,按照业主大会的决定履行管理的职责。
《五条誓文》
一个2×3×4的完全随机多因素设计可以得到几个交互作用?()
Shestumbledand________thecoffee.
最新回复
(
0
)